
Biology Forum 91, 57-74 (1998)
by Rupert Sheldrake, Catherine Lawlor and Jane Turney
1. Can Pets Be Telepathic?
Pet owners often comment on the surprising perceptiveness of their animals. For example, some cats seem to pick up when their owners intend to take them to the vet, and disappear, even when the person has tried to give the cat no clue. And some dogs seem to know when their owners are about to return, half an hour or more in advance, even when the person comes at an unusual time or in an unfamiliar vehicle (Sheldrake [1994]).
Such phenomena have, so far, been neglected by biologists, ethologists, psychologists, veterinarians and even by parapsychologists. One reason for this neglect may be the widespread taboo among scientists against taking seemingly 'paranormal' phenomena seriously. Another may be the taboo against taking pets seriously (Serpell [1986]). The scientific understanding of animal behaviour has so far been based mainly on laboratory experiments and on the observation of animals in the wild, or in farms and zoos. In these kinds of research, investigators typically try to avoid emotional connections with the animals they are investigating; they aspire to a detached objectivity. They would therefore be unlikely to encounter kinds of behaviour that depend on close attachments between person and animal. In this realm, pet keepers are generally more knowledgeable and experienced than professional researchers on animal behaviour.
In order to assess how common the claims of pet owners are about the perceptiveness of their pets, it is necessary to survey randomly sampled households. One such survey has already been carried out in Greater Manchester, England (Sheldrake and Smart [1997]), and another in Santa Cruz, California, USA (Brown and Sheldrake [1998]). Here we describe the the final survey of this series, in London, England. We used the same questionnaire as the previous two surveys so that the results can be compared.
Of course, what people believe about their pets' abilities may not be true. But it may not be false either. Only empirical investigation can shed further light on the seemingly unexplained powers of pets investigated in this survey.
2. The Telephonic Survey
This survey was conducted by telephone by Jane Turney (JT) and Catherine Lawlor (CL) in London between November 1996 and September 1997. Households were selected from the British Telecom Telephone Directory 616, North London Residential, by JT, and from Telephone Directory 614, East London Residential, by CL. Random number tables were used to determine the page numbers and the line numbers, and the column was selected by throwing a die.
JT and CL introduced themselves as follows: "My name is Jane /Cathy. I'm doing research on unexplained powers of animals, and would like to ask you a few questions" Approximately 75% of the people reached by phone agreed to take part in the survey. JT or CL then asked a series of questions and recorded the answers on a standard form as follows.
Name: ______________________________
Tel: ___________________________
Address: ___________________________________________________________
1. Do you or anyone in your household own a pet? |
Yes |
No |
2. What type of animal? Species: |
3. Have you/they ever noticed the pet getting agitated before a family member has arrived home? |
Yes |
No |
4. How long before you/they arrive is your pet agitated? | |||
0-5 mins | 5-10 mins | 10-20 mins | 20 mins or more |
5. Would you agree or disagree that your pet
knows you are going out before you show any physical signs of doing so? | |||
Agree | Disagree | Don't know |
6. Would you agree or disagree that you pet
responds to your own thoughts or silent commands? | |||
Agree | Disagree | Don't know |
7. Would you agree or disagree that your pet is sometimes telepathic with you? | |||
Agree | Disagree | Don't know |
8. Would you agree or disagree that any of the pets you have known in the past were telepathic? | |||
Agree | Disagree | Don't know |
9. How frequently have you yourself had what you would consider to be a psychical experience? | |||
Never | Sometimes | Frequently |
In cases where respondents currently had no pets (i.e., they answered 'no' to the first question), they were only asked Questions 8 and 9.
In response to question 3, people living alone often replied "don't know". Such answers were treated as "no". Hence the number of pets anticipating their owners' arrival may have been underestimated.
In Tables 1 to 4, where dogs and cats are compared, responses from people who kept both dogs and cats are included in both totals. In most cases their replies referred to their dog rather than their cat, and when no information was given about the other pet, it was classified as "no" for question 3 and "don't know" for questions 5-7.
Statistical analysis was carried out using 2 x 2 contingency tables and the chi-squared test (Campbell [1989]). Probability values for two-tailed tests were used. For the comparison of data for dogs and cats in Table 2, and for the comparison of 'psychic' and 'non-psychic' owners in Table 4, 'don't know' answers were ignored.
3. Pet Ownership
Out of 387 households surveyed, 180 (46.5%) had pets, slightly below the UK national average of 50% (Pet Food Manufacturers' Association, 1995). Cats were the most common pet, followed by dogs. The number of households with the following species were:
Cats | 95 | Horses | 2 |
Dogs | 63 | Tortoises | 2 |
Rabbits | 9 | Sheep | 1 |
Fish | 9 | Ferret | 1 |
Birds | 7 | Guinea Pig | 1 |
Hamsters | 5 | Terrapin | 1 |
Gerbils | 2 |
|
Most of these households had one kind of pet: 84 had cats only, and 51 had dogs only; 8 had both dogs and cats; 3 had cats and other pets (excluding dogs); 4 had dogs and other pets; and 30 had only other pets.
These London households were unusual in that more had cats than dogs, whereas nationally more households have dogs than cats. A lower proportion of households had dogs (16%) than the U.K. average of 23% and a higher proportion had cats (25%) than the average of 21% (Pet Food Manufacturers' Association, 1995). The relatively low proportion of dogs reflects a general tendency for dog ownership to be less in large cities, where more people live in flats, than in small towns, suburbs and rural areas, where more people live in houses.
4. Dogs and Cats that Seem to Anticipate the Arrival of a Member of the Household
The great majority of animals said to anticipate their owner's arrival were dogs and cats, but one rabbit and one ferret were also said to do so. Data for dogs and cats in reply to Questions 3 and 4 are shown in Table 1.
Table 1. Replies to Questions 3 and 4
Anticipation of owners's arrival | ||||
Numbers (and percentages) of dogs or cats
| ||||
Total |
Anticipation |
No anticipation |
||
DOGS |
63 |
33 (52%) |
30 (48%) |
|
CATS |
95 |
22 (23%) |
73 (77%) |
Period of anticipation: minutes before
arrival | |||||
Numbers (and percentages) of dogs or cats
reacting | |||||
Total | 0-5 min | 5-10 min | 10-20 min | 20+ min | |
DOGS | 33 | 20 (61%) | 6 (18%) | 5 (15%) | 2 (6%) |
CATS | 22 | 12 (54%) | 6 (27%) | 1 (5%) | 3 (14%) |
A far higher proportion of dogs than cats appears to anticipate arrivals. In this survey the figures were 52% and 23% respectively. This difference was highly significant statistically (p<0.0005).
Most pets that were said to anticipate arrivals did so less than five minutes beforehand, but 21% of dog owners and 19% of cat owners said that the animal showed anticipatory behaviour more than ten minutes before the household members' arrival.
5. Pets That Seem to Respond to Their Owners' Thoughts and Intentions
Pet owners often report that their animals seem to know when they are going out before they show any physical signs of doing so. Some also claim that their pets can actually respond directly to their thoughts or silent intentions, or are telepathic with them. Questions 5, 6 and 7 were asked to find out how common these impressions are. The results for dogs and cats are summarized in Table 2. In addition, one budgerigar, one canary and one cockatiel were said to respond to their owners thoughts and silent commands, and one rabbit and one ferret were said to be telepathic with their owners.
Table 2. Replies to Questions 5, 6 and 7.
Numbers (and percentages) of dogs and cats. The total number
of dogs was 63 and of cats 95
A. Anticipation of owner's departure | ||||
Anticipation | No anticipation | Don't Know | ||
DOGS | 46 (73%) | 14 (22%) | 3 (5%) | |
CATS | 45 (47%) | 40 (42%) | 10 (11%) | |
B. Responses to owner's thoughts and silent commands | ||||
Response | No response | Don't know | ||
DOGS | 27 (43%) | 21 (33%) | 15 (24%) | |
CATS | 36 (38%) | 48 (51%) | 11 (12%) | |
C. Telepathy with owner | ||||
Telepathy | No telepathy | Don't know | ||
DOGS | 37 (39%) | 16 (25%) | 10 (16%) | |
CATS | 33 (35%) | 50 (53%) | 12 (13%) |
Dog owners agreed more than cat owners (73% and 47% respectively) that their pet knows that they are going out before they show any physical signs of doing so. This difference was statistically significant (p<0.005).
Slightly more dog than cat owners (43% and 38% respectively) agreed that their pet responds to their thoughts or silent commands, but this difference was not significant statistically.
More dog than cat owners (59% and 35% respectively) believed that their pet was telepathic with them, and this difference was statistically significant (p<0.001).
6. Telepathic Connections with Pets in the Past
Both pet owners and non-pet owners were asked about telepathy in pets they have known in the past, and the results are summarized in Table 3. 46% of current pet owners, and 36% of non-pet owners said that they had known pets in the past that they considered to be telepathic. This difference was not statistically significant.
7. The Respondents' Own Psychic Experience
In our survey, 39% of pet owners and 38% of people without pets said they had had psychic experiences themselves (Table 3). These figures are lower than those in a similar survey in North-West England, 54% (Sheldrake and Smart [1997]), and by other surveys in Britain: 64% (Haraldsson [1985] and 54% (Gaynard [1992]). We do not know why a relatively low proportion of the people in this survey gave positive answers.
Among non-pet owners, there was a remarkable difference between people who had kept pets in the past and those who had not: 38% of those with pets in the past claimed psychical experiences, compared with only 8% of those who had never kept pets (Table 3). This difference was highly significant statistically (p<0.0005).
Table 3 Replies to Questions 8 and 9: Replies to
Questions 8 and 9: Comparison of present pet owners and people with
no pets now in relation to (A) telepathy with past pets and (B) the
person's own psychic experience.
Numbers (and percentages) of people
A. Telepathy with past pets
Total |
Telepathy |
No telepathy |
Don't know | |
PETS NOW |
180 |
82 (46%) |
68 (38%) |
30 (17%) |
NO PETS NOW |
207 |
75 (36%) |
69 (33%) |
63 (30%) |
B. Frequency of people's own psychic experiences
Total |
Frequently |
Sometimes |
Never | |
PETS NOW* |
180 |
11 (6%) |
59 (33%) |
110 (61%) |
Dog owners |
63 |
3 (5%) |
22 (35%) |
38 (60%) |
Cat owners |
95 |
5 (5%) |
33 (35%) |
57 (60%) |
NO PETS NOW** |
207 |
12 (6%) |
66 (32%) |
129 (62%) |
Pets in Past |
123 |
7 (6%) |
39 (32%) |
77 (63%) |
No Pets Ever |
38 |
0 |
3 (8%) |
35 (92%) |
* "Pets now" refers to all people currently keeping pets, of whom some had neither dogs nor cats, but other animals. This is why the total of dog and cat owners comes to 158 rather than180.
** Not all the 207 people with no pets now were asked whether they had had pets in the past; only 161 were asked this question. That is why those with "pets in past" and "no pets ever" add up to 161 not 207.
The proportion of dog and cat owners who said they had had psychic experiences was the same, 40%.
We compared the answers about their pets given by dog and cat owners who said they had themselves had psychic experiences frequently or sometimes ('psychic owners') with the answers given by owners who said they had never had psychic experiences ('non-psychic owners'). For all questions, and for both dogs and cats, psychic owners gave more positive responses than non-psychic owners (Table 4). Most of these differences were statistically significant, with the notable exception of Question 3, about pets anticipating homecomings. The most significant differences were in response to the questions about telepathy with present and past pets.
In brief, more psychic owners said their pets were psychic than non-psychic owners.
8. How Reliable Are These Findings?
As in other surveys based on questionnaires or interviews, the answers may have been subject to a variety of biases. Here are three possibilities:
- The way the questions were asked may have encouraged respondents to give positive answers in an attempt to please the interviewer. Conversely, people may have been reluctant to give positive answers about their pets and about themselves when they were talking on the telephone to a stranger. Nevetheless, these possible sources of bias cannot account for the striking differences in answers about dogs and cats.
- People who are very fond of their pets may have exaggerated their animals' powers, or been victims of wishful thinking. Conversely, people who pay little attention to their animals may not have noticed their perceptiveness. And animals may respond less to people who show scant interest in them. But again, these possible biases cannot explain why the reports about the behaviour of dogs and cats were so different.
- People who believe in psychic phenomena may be more likely to say their pet is psychic. Conversely, people who disbelieve in psychic phenomena may be less inclined to notice seemingly psychic responses in their pets. This possibility is supported by our finding that 'psychic' owners generally gave more positive responses to questions about their pets than 'non-psychic' owners, and most of these differences were significant statistically (Table 4). Nevertheless, the general pattern of responses by both groups was the same. More dogs than cats were said to know when their owners were about to return, about to leave, and to be telepathic. And even among 'non-psychic' owners, 45% of dogs and 23% of cats were said to know when their owners were about to return; 39% and 28% respectively were said to respond to thoughts and silent commands, and 42% and 25% were said to be telepathic with their owners.
Numbers (and percentages) of people giving positive responses, and the statistical significance of differences between psychic and non-psychic owners.
A. DOGS | |||
Psychic |
Non-psychic |
Significance (p) | |
Totals |
25 |
38 |
|
Know when arriving |
16 (64%) |
17 (45%) |
NS |
Know when leaving |
22 (88%) |
24 (63%) |
<0.05 |
Silent commands |
12 (48%) |
15 (39%) |
NS |
Telepathy |
21 (84%) |
16 (42%) |
<0.001 |
Telepathy: past pets |
18 (72%) |
11 (29%) |
<0.001 |
B. CATS | |||
Psychic |
Non-psychic |
Significance(p) | |
Totals |
38 |
57 |
|
Know when arriving |
9 (24%) |
13 (23%) |
NS |
Know when leaving |
23 (61%) |
22 (39%) |
<0.05 |
Silent commands |
20 (53%) |
16 (28%) |
<0.02 |
Telepathy |
19 (50%) |
14 (25%) |
<0.02 |
Telepathy: past pets |
20 (53%) |
18 (32%) |
<0.05 |
Nevertheless, in spite of such possible sources of bias, the general pattern of results was quite consistent, and leads to two main conclusions. First, perceptive pets are common. And second, more dogs than cats are responsive to their owners' intentions and anticipate their comings and goings.
How repeatable are these findings? Fortunately, we can answer this question by comparing the results of this survey in London with previous surveys carried out in in very different physical, social and cultural environments, and conducted by different interviewers.
9. Comparison with Previous Surveys
This survey in London complements two similar surveys in Ramsbottom (Sheldrake and Smart [1997]), a town in Greater Manchester, England and in Santa Cruz (Brown and Sheldrake [1998]), a university and beach town in California, USA. In most respects, the surveys are in remarkable agreement. Summaries of all three are shown in Tables 5 and 6. These are their principal features:
- Many owners said they had noticed perceptive behaviour in their dogs and cats about their impending returns and departures (Table 4). Between 45% and 52% of dog owners and between 14% and 31% of cat owners said their animal anticipated the arrival of a member of the household.
- Between 42% and 59% of dog owners and between 25% and 35% of cat owners said that their pet was sometimes telepathic with them (Table 4).
- These claims about perceptive behaviour nearly all concerned dogs and cats. No hamsters, goldfish, gerbils, stick insects, tortoises or chickens were said to show any of these kinds of responses. Taking all three surveys together, four birds out of a total of 30 were said to anticipate their owners' arrivals (a parrot, a parakeet, a cockatoo and a cockatiel) , one rabbit out of 32, and a ferret.
- At all locations, dogs were said to be more responsive than cats to their owners' impending returns (dogs, 45-52%; cats, 14-31%) and departures (dogs, 65-73%; cats, 32-47%). In most cases the differences between dogs and cats were very significant statistically. But cats may not be less sensitive than dogs; they may merely take less interest in their owners' comings and goings, owing to their more independent nature (Hart [1995]).
- In relation to thoughts, silent commands and telepathy with their owners, once again at all locations dogs appeared to be more responsive than cats.
- About half the people who currently own pets in all three locations said they had known telepathic pets in the past (Table 6). Among people who do not currently own pets, the proportion who said they had known telepathic pets in the past ranged from 31% in Santa Cruz to 55% in Ramsbottom.
- At all three locations, significantly more 'psychic' pet owners said that their pets showed perceptive behaviour than 'non-psychic' pet owners. Nevertheless, the general pattern of response from non-psychic pet owners was similar to that of psychic pet owners; but the percentages were lower (the London data are in Table 4).
-
Table 5 Comparison of surveys in London, Ramsbottom and
Santa Cruz
Percentage of positive answers to Question1
households with pets3
know when returning4
>10 mins before5
know when going out6
silent commands7
telepathyDOGS London 165221734359Ramsbottom 314616695354Santa Cruz 354519654642CATS London 252319473835Ramsbottom 241423322325Santa Cruz 423122374134
Table 6Comparison of pet owners and people without pets in London, Ramsbottom and Santa CruzPercentage of positive answers to questions:
8
Past pets telepathic9
Own psychic experiencePETS NOW London 4639Ramsbottom 5354Santa Cruz 4964NO PETS NOW London 3638Ramsbottom 55-*Santa Cruz 3140*non-pet owners were not asked this question in the Ramsbottom survey.
10. The Need for Experimental Investigations
The results of these surveys do not necessarily imply the existence of a 'sixth sense' or telepathy in animals. But they certainly show that many pet owners say that their animals are telepathic.
Some of the seemingly mysterious phenomena discussed in this paper may ultimately be explicable in terms of the impressive sensory range of cats and dogs, combined with subtle cues of which their owners are unaware. However, some may be due to influences at present unknown to science. Experimental investigations are needed to tease apart these possibilities.
In the case of a dog that appears to anticipate his owner's arrival, experiments have already shown that this response does not seem explicable in terms of routine, the sounds of familiar vehicles or other auditory cues, or knowledge by the people at home when the owner will return (Sheldrake and Smart [1998]). An influence currently unknown to science may indeed be at work. These investigations are continuing.
There has so far been almost no scientific research on the seeming ability of some dogs and cats to pick up their owners' thoughts and intentions. This subject has been ignored by biologists and by parapsychologists alike. No doubt some of the animals' perceptiveness can be expained in terms of gross or subtle cues, including people's unconscious body language. But can it all be explained like this? The only way to find out is to separate the pet and owner, so that communication by sight, smell and hearing can be ruled out. Can the animal still respond to the owner's intentions, even at non-routine times? No one knows. This is a virgin field of research.
11. Implications for Biology and Psychology
If some pets do turn out to have unexplained powers, there would be several far-reaching implications. First, the boundaries of scientific explanation would have to be widened, for example by hypothesizing a new kind of field connecting pets and owners, or another kind of interaction at present unknown to physics, or by extending the idea of non-locality and non-separability already present in quantum physics (Sheldrake [1994]; Sermonti [1996] and Sheldrake [1996]). An extensive discussion of these possibilities has recently been published in German (Dürr and Gottwald [1997]).
Second, if present in pets, similar forms of perceptiveness may play an important role in communication between animals in the wild, and on farms.
Third, if animals have a 'sixth sense' or telepathic powers, then the evidence for such phenomena in human beings (e.g. Radin [1997]) would take on a new significance. These phenomena would no longer be seen as peculiarly human, but as rooted in our biological heritage. They may be widespread and better-developed in many species of animals than they are in people. And perhaps civilized people have lost them more than most.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to all the people who took part in this survey. We thank the Lifebridge Foundation, New York and the Institute of Noetic Sciences, Sausalito, California, for their financial support.
References
Brown, D and Sheldrake, R. [1998] Perceptive pets: A survey in California. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research (in the press)
Campbell, R.C. [1989] Statistics for Biologists. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Dürr, H.P. and Gottwald, F.T. (eds) [1997] Rupert Sheldrake in der Diskussion: Das Wagnis einer neuen Wissenschaft des Lebens. Scherz, Bern.
Haraldsson, E. [1985] Representative national surveys of psychic phenomena. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research53: 137-146.
Gaynard, T.J. [1992] Young people and the paranormal. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research 58: 165-180.
Hart, L.A. [1995] Dogs as human companions: a review of the relationship. In: The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People , ed. J. Serpell. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Pet Food Manufacturers' Association [1995] PFMA Profile 1995. Pet Food Manufacturers' Association, London.
Radin, D. [1997] The Conscious Universe: The Scientific Truth of Psychic Phenomena. HarperCollins, San Francisco.
Sermonti, G. [1996] The impossible exists: About the 'Seven Experiments' suggested by Rupert Sheldrake. Biology Forum 89: 479-482.
Serpell, J. [1986] In the Company of Animals. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Sheldrake, R. [1994] Seven Experiments that Could Change the World. Fourth Estate, London.
Sheldrake, R. [1996] Impossible.... for the current physics: Reply to the open letter by Giuseppe Sermonti. Biology Forum 89: 483-486.
Sheldrake, R. and Smart, P. [1997] Psychic pets: a survey in North-West England. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research 61: 353-64.
Sheldrake, R. and Smart, P. [1998] A dog that seems to know when his owner is returning: Preliminary investigations. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research 62: 220-232.